There Is No Such Thing as Race
In 1950, the United Nations Educational,
Scientific and Cultural Organization (UNESCO) issued a statement
asserting that all humans belong to the same species and that “race” is
not a biological reality but a myth. This was a summary of the findings
of an international panel of anthropologists, geneticists, sociologists,
and psychologists.
A great deal of evidence had accumulated by that
time to support this conclusion, and the scientists involved were those
who were conducting research and were most knowledgeable about the topic
of human variation. Since that time similar statements have been
published by the American Anthropological Association and the American
Association of Physical Anthropologists, and an enormous amount of
modern scientific data has been gathered to justify this conclusion.
Today the vast majority of those involved in
research on human variation would agree that biological races do not
exist among humans. Among those who study the subject, who use and
accept modern scientific techniques and logic, this scientific fact is
as valid and true as the fact that the earth is round and revolves
around the sun.
Yet as recently as 2010, highly acclaimed journalist Guy Harrison wrote:
One day in the 1980s, I sat in the front row in my
first undergraduate anthropology class, eager to learn more about this
bizarre and fascinating species I was born into. But I got more than I
expected that day as I heard for the first time that biological races
are not real. After hearing several perfectly sensible reasons why vast
biological categories don’t work very well, I started to feel betrayed
by my society. “Why am I just hearing this now? . . . Why didn’t
somebody tell me this in elementary school?” . . . I never should have
made it through twelve years of schooling before entering a university,
without ever hearing the important news that most anthropologists reject
the concept of biological races.
Unfortunately, along with the belief in the
reality of biologically based human races, racism still abounds in the
United States and Western Europe. How can this be when there is so much
scientific evidence against it?
Most educated people would accept the facts that
the earth is not flat and that it revolves around the sun. However, it
is much more difficult for them to accept modern science concerning
human variation. Why is this so?
It seems that the belief in human races, carrying
along with it the prejudice and hatred of “racism,” is so embedded in
our culture and has been an integral part of our worldview for so long
that many of us assume that it just must be true.
Racism is a part of our everyday lives. Where you
live, where you go to school, your job, your profession, who you
interact with, how people interact with you, your treatment in the
healthcare and justice systems are all affected by your race.
For the past 500 years, people have been taught
how to interpret and understand racism. We have been told that there are
very specific things that relate to race, such as intelligence, sexual
behavior, birth rates, infant care, work ethics and abilities, personal
restraint, lifespan, law-abidingness, aggression, altruism, economic and
business practices, family cohesion, and even brain size.
We have learned that races are structured in a
hierarchical order and that some races are better than others. Even if
you are not a racist, your life is affected by this ordered structure.
We are born into a racist society.
What many people do not realize is that this
racial structure is not based on reality. Anthropologists have shown for
many years now that there is no biological reality to human race. There
are no major complex behaviors that directly correlate with what might
be considered human “racial” characteristics.
There is no inherent relationship between
intelligence, law- abidingness, or economic practices and race, just as
there is no relationship between nose size, height, blood group, or skin
color and any set of complex human behaviors.
However, over the past 500 years, we have been
taught by an informal, mutually reinforcing consortium of intellectuals,
politicians, statesmen, business and economic leaders and their books
that human racial biology is real and that certain races are
biologically better than others.
These teachings have led to major injustices to
Jews and non-Christians during the Spanish Inquisition; to blacks,
Native Americans, and others during colonial times; to African Americans
during slavery and reconstruction; to Jews and other Europeans during
the reign of the Nazis in Germany; and to groups from Latin America and
the Middle East, among others, during modern political times.
In my book, The Myth of Race: The Troubling Persistence of an Unscientific Idea,
I have not dwelt upon all of the scientific information that has been
gathered by anthropologists, biologists, geneticists, and other
scientists concerning the fact that there are no such things as human
biological races. This has been done by many people over the past fifty
or so years.
What I do is describe the history of our myth of
race and racism. As I describe this history, I think that you will be
able to understand why many of our leaders and their followers have
deluded us into believing these racist fallacies and how they have been
perpetuated from the late Middle Ages to the present.
Many of our basic policies of race and racism have
been developed as a way to keep these leaders and their followers in
control of the way we live our modern lives. These leaders often see
themselves as the best and the brightest. Much of this history helped
establish and maintain the Spanish Inquisition, colonial policies,
slavery, Nazism, racial separatism and discrimination, and
anti-immigration policies.
Although policies related to racism seem to be
improving over time, I hope to help clarify why this myth still exists
and remains widespread in the United States and throughout Western
Europe by describing the history of racism and by exploring how the
anthropological concepts of culture and worldview have challenged and
disproven the validity of racist views.
Over the past 500 or so years, many intellectuals
and their books have created our story of racism. They developed our
initial ideas of race in Western society and solidified the attitudes
and beliefs that gradually followed under the influence of their
economic and political policies.
Then, approximately 100 years ago, anthropologist
Franz Boas came up with an alternate explanation for why peoples from
different areas or living under certain conditions behaved differently
from one another. People have divergent life histories, different shared
experiences with distinctive ways of relating to these differences. We
all have a worldview, and we all share our worldview with others with
similar experiences. We have culture.
It took many years for Boas and his few followers
to develop this idea and pass it on to others. However, over the past
fifty or sixty years, anthropologists, biologists, and geneticists have
written many articles and books explaining why biological race in humans
is nonexistent.
At first, scientists attempted to classify human
races based on variations in characteristics such as skin color, hair
color and form, eye color, facial anatomy, and blood groups. In the
recent past, various scientists, such as Franz Boas, have divided us
into anywhere between three and more than thirty different races,
without any success. Most of these hypothetical “races” were developed
using assumptions about genetic relationships and distributions among
different human populations.
In 1942, Ashley Montagu, a student of Franz Boas,
claimed that “there are no races, there are only clines.” Traits
considered to be “racial” are actually distributed independently and
depend upon many environmental and behavioral factors. For the most
part, each trait has a distinct distribution from other traits, and
these traits are rarely determined by a single genetic factor.
This type of distribution of a biological trait is
referred to as a cline. For example, skin color is related to the
amount of solar radiation, and dark skin is found in Africa, India, and
Australia. However, many other genetic traits in peoples of these areas
are not similar. Furthermore, similar traits such as skin color are
convergent; different genes can cause similar morphological and
behavioral characteristics.
For example, genetic pathways to dark skin are
different in Tamil Nadu and in Nigeria. Genetic traits usually do not
correlate with one another and are not distributed in the same place or
in the same way over time.
Race is supposed to tell us something about our
genetic history. Who is related to whom? How did populations evolve over
time and how isolated were they in the past?
Recent studies have shown us that humans have been
migrating since Homo sapiens evolved some 200,000 years ago. This
migration has not been in one direction but had happened back and forth.
Our genes have been mixing since we evolved, and our genetic structure
looks more like a complex, intermixed trellis than a simple candelabra.
It is very difficult to tell what our particular
genetic background is over human historic time. We humans are more
similar to each other as a group than we are to one another within any
particular racial or genetic category. Many anthropological books have
been written to explain this phenomenon.
Our view of genetics has also changed in recent
times. Although many people still believe that genes, or a series of
genes, directly determine some of our most complex behavioral or
cognitive characteristics, the reality is more complicated.
Studies now show that each gene is only a single
player in a wondrous, intricate drama involving non-additive
interactions of genes, proteins, hormones, food, and life experiences
and learning that interact to affect us on different levels of cognitive
and behavioral functions. Each gene has an effect on multiple types of
behaviors, and many behaviors are affected by many genes as well as
other factors. The assumption that a single gene is causative can lead
to unwarranted conclusions and an over- interpretation of any genuine
genetic linkage.
Before beginning this story, however, it is
important to understand how scientists define the concept of race. How
is race defined in biological terms? What do we mean by the term race
when describing population variation in large mammals such as humans? Do
the criteria used in describing these variations hold when we examine
human population variation?
In biological terms, the concept of race is
integrally bound to the process of evolution and the origin of species.
It is part of the process of the formation of new species and is related
to subspecific differentiation. However, because conditions can change
and subspecies can and do merge, this process does not necessarily lead
to the development of new species.
In biology, a species is defined as a population
of individuals who are able to mate and have viable offspring; that is,
offspring who are also successful in reproducing. The formation of new
species usually occurs slowly over a long period of time.
For example, many species have a widespread
geographic distribution with ranges that include ecologically diverse
regions. If these regions are large in relationship to the average
distance of migration of individuals within the species, there will be
more mating, and thus more exchange of genes, within than between
regions.
Over very long periods of time (tens of thousands
of years), differences would be expected to evolve between distant
populations of the same species. Some of these variations would be
related to adaptations to ecological differences within the geographic
range of the populations, while others might be purely random.
Over time, if little or no mating (or genetic
exchange) occurs between these distant populations, genetic (and related
morphological) differences will increase. Ultimately, over tens of
thousands of years of separation, if little or no mating takes place
between separate populations, genetic distinctions can become so great
that individuals of the different populations could no longer mate and
produce viable offspring.
The two populations would now be considered two
separate species. This is the process of speciation. However, again,
none of these criteria require that speciation will ultimately occur.
Since speciation develops very slowly, it is
useful to recognize intermediate stages in this process. Populations of a
species undergoing differentiation would show genetic and morphological
variation due to a buildup of genetic differences but would still be
able to breed and have offspring that could successfully reproduce.
They would be in various stages of the process of
speciation but not yet different species. In biological terminology, it
is these populations that are considered “races” or “subspecies”.
Basically, subspecies within a species are geographically,
morphologically, and genetically distinct populations but still maintain
the possibility of successful interbreeding.
Thus, using this biological definition of race, we
assume that races or subspecies are populations of a species that have
genetic and morphological differences due to barriers to mating.
Furthermore, little or no mating (or genetic exchange) between them has
persisted for extremely long periods of time, thus giving the
individuals within the population a common and separate evolutionary
history.
Given advances in molecular genetics, we now have
the ability to examine populations of species and subspecies and
reconstruct their evolutionary histories in an objective and explicit
fashion. In this way, we can determine the validity of the traditional
definition of human races “by examining the patterns and amount of
genetic diversity found within and among human populations” and by
comparing this diversity with other large-bodied mammals that have wide
geographic distributions.
In other words, we can determine how much populations of a species differ from one another and how these divergences came about.
A commonly used method to quantify the amount of
within -- to among -- group genetic diversity is through examining
molecular data, using statistics measuring genetic differences within
and between populations of a species. Using this method, biologists have
set a minimal threshold for the amount of genetic differentiation that
is required to recognize subspecies.
Compared to other large mammals with wide
geographic distributions, human populations do not reach this threshold.
In fact, even though humans have the widest distribution, the measure
of human genetic diversity (based on sixteen populations from Europe,
Africa, Asia, the Americas, and the Australia-Pacific region) falls well
below the threshold used to recognize races for other species and is
among the lowest value known for large mammalian species. This is true
even if we compare humans to chimpanzees.
Using a number of molecular markers has shown that
the degree of isolation among human populations that would have been
necessary for the formation of biological subspecies or races never
occurred during the 200,000 years of modern human evolution.
Combined genetic data reveal that from around one
million years ago to the last tens of thousands of years, human
evolution has been dominated by two evolutionary forces: (1) constant
population movement and range expansion; and (2) restrictions on mating
between individuals only because of distance.
Thus, there is no evidence of fixed, long-term
geographic isolation between populations. Other than some rare,
temporary isolation events, such as the isolation of the aborigines of
Australia, for example, the major human populations have been
interconnected by mating opportunities (and thus genetic mixture) during
the last 200,000 years (as long as modern humans, Homo sapiens, have
been around). As summarized by A.R. Templeton, who is among the world’s
most recognized and respected geneticists:
Because of the extensive evidence for genetic
interchange through population movements and recurrent gene flow going
back at least hundreds of thousands of years ago, there is only one
evolutionary lineage of humanity and there are no subspecies or races. .
. . Human evolution and population structure has been and is
characterized by many locally differentiated populations coexisting at
any given time, but with sufficient contact to make all of humanity a
single lineage sharing a common, long-term evolutionary fate.
Thus, given current scientific data, biological
races do not exist among modern humans today, and they have never
existed in the past. Given such clear scientific evidence as this and
the research data of so many other biologists, anthropologists, and
geneticists that demonstrate the nonexistence of biological races among
humans, how can the “myth” of human races still persist?
If races do not exist as a biological
reality, why do so many people still believe that they do? In fact, even
though biological races do not exist, the concept of race obviously is
still a reality, as is racism. These are prevalent and persistent
elements of our everyday lives and generally accepted aspects of our culture.
Thus, the concept of human races is real. It is
not a biological reality, however, but a cultural one. Race is not a
part of our biology, but it is definitely a part of our culture. Race
and racism are deeply ingrained in our history.
From The Myth of Race: The Troubling Persistence of an Unscientific Idea by
Robert Wald Sussman. Copyright © 2014 by the President and Fellows of
Harvard College. Used by permission. All rights reserved.
No comments:
Post a Comment